Issue 34
Vanishing Points

The Isolated Frame as Subliminal Factor: Analysis and Categorization

Javier Sanz Aznar
Universidad de Barcelona
Bio
Juan José Caballero Molina
Universitat de Barcelona
Bio

Published 2022-07-01

Keywords

  • Subliminal,
  • Cinema,
  • Film,
  • Conditioning,
  • Frame,
  • Spectator,
  • Psychology.
  • ...More
    Less

Abstract

Among the possible subliminal stimuli that a film may contain, the focus of this study is on those that are viewed for such a short time that they do not enter the spectator’s conscious mind. This occurs when the viewing time of the stimulus does not exceed the subjective threshold but is still long enough to have an impact on the spectator, triggering certain neural processes that influence the reception of subsequent supraliminal stimuli. The on-screen duration of this stimulus must necessarily be limited to a single still frame, as the subjective threshold will be exceeded if there is more than one. For this reason, it is not possible to speak of a “subliminal shot,” but only of a “subliminal frame.” This fleeting frame has no influence on the narrative of the film, as it does not have any time span, nor is it capable of influencing the narrative or dramatic development of the film; however, based on the cognitive and emotional impact it can have on the images that follow them, subliminal frames can be distinguished and categorised according to their nature and characteristics, with a view to determining their cognitive effects, and formulating useful parameters for their design, function, and location.

References

  1. Altman, R. (1996). Otra forma de pensar la historia (del cine): un modelo de crisis. Archivos de la Filmoteca, 22, 6-19.
  2. Armstrong, A. M., Dienes, Z. (2013). Subliminal understanding of negation: Unconscious control by subliminal processing of word pairs. Consciousness and cognition, 22(3), 1022-1040. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2013.06.010
  3. Aumont, J. (2000). Le plan. Cahiers du cinéma, número especial: Le siècle du cinéma, 38-39.
  4. Aumont, J., Vernet, M., Marie, M., Bergala, A. (1985). Estética del cine: espacio fílmico, montaje, narración, lenguaje. Barcelona: Paidós.
  5. Berkovitch, L., Dehaene, S. (2019). Subliminal syntactic priming. Cognitive psychology, 109, 26-46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2018.12.001
  6. Bonitzer, P. (1995). La métamorphose. En P. Bonitzer (ed.), Décadrages: peinture et cinéma (pp. 87-92). París: Cahiers du cinéma.
  7. Broyles, S. (2006). Subliminal advertising and the perpetual popularity of playing to people’s paranoia. Journal of Consumer Affairs, 40(2), 392-406. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6606.2006.00063.x
  8. Cheesman, J., Merikle, P. (1984). Priming with and without awareness. Perception & psychophysics, 36(4), 387-395. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03202793
  9. Cohn, N., Kutas, M. (2017). What’s your neural function, visual narrative conjunction? Grammar, meaning, and fluency in sequential image processing. Cognitive Research, 2(1), 27. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-017-0064-5
  10. Conway, M., Csank, P., Holm, S., Blake, C. (2000). On assessing individual differences in rumination on sadness. Journal of personality assessment, 75(3), 404-425. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327752JPA7503_04
  11. Crick, F., Koch, C. (1990). Towards a neurobiological theory of consciousness. Seminars in the Neurosciences, 2, 263-275.
  12. Crittenden, R. (1991). The Thames and Hudson Manual of Film Editing. Londres: Thames & Hudson.
  13. Epstein, J. (1988). A propósito de algunas condiciones de la fotogenia. En J. Romagueira i Ramió y H. Alsina Thevenet (eds.), Textos y manifiestos del cine: estética, escuelas, movimientos, disciplinas, innovaciones (pp. 40-46). Buenos Aires: Corregidor.
  14. García Matilla, E. (1990). Subliminal: escrito en nuestro cerebro. Madrid: Bitácora.
  15. Lai, C., Pellicano, G., Ciacchella, C., Guidobaldi, L., Altavilla, D., Cecchini, M., Begotaraj, E., Aceto, P., Luciani, M. (2020). Neurophysiological correlates of emotional face perception consciousness. Neuropsychologia, 146, 107.554. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107554
  16. Liddell, B., Brown, K., Kemp, A., Barton, M., Das, P., Peduto, A., Gordon, E., Williams, L. (2005). A direct brainstem–amygdala–cortical ‘alarm’ system for subliminal signals of fear. NeuroImage, 24(1), 235-243. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.016
  17. Liu, J., Pan, W., Xiong, D., Lin, R., Du, J., Ye, J., Zhou, Y., Cheng, Q., Yang, L., Zhang, Y., Bai, S. (2020). An Experimental Study of the Effects of Subliminal Stimulation on Attention Perception. En S. Long, y B. Dhillon (coord.), International Conference on Man-Machine-Environment System Engineering (pp. 163-169). Singapur: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-15-6978-4_20
  18. Marimón, J. (2015). El muntatge cinematogràfic. Del guió a la pantalla. Barcelona: Universitat de Barcelona.
  19. McLuhan, M. (1996). Comprender los medios de comunicación. Barcelona: Paidós.
  20. Milner, D., Goodale, M. (2006). The visual brain in action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  21. Molina, E. C. (2012a). Apuntes sobre la percepción subconsciente en el cine: el ejemplo de Alien, el octavo pasajero, el octavo pasajero (1979) y su propuesta orgánica de atracción/repulsión. Revista Mediterránea de Comunicación, 3(2), 46-82. https://doi.org/10.14198/MEDCOM2012.3.09
  22. Molina, E. C. (2012b). El impulso de la publicidad viral cinematográfica: Prometheus (Ridley Scott, 2012) y su ensayo ‘transmediático’. Historia y Comunicación Social, 18, 89-103. http://dx.doi.org/10.5209/rev_HICS.2013.v18.44314
  23. Pan, F., Wu, X., Zhang, L., Ou, Y. (2017). Inhibition of return is modulated by negative stimuli: evidence from subliminal perception. Frontiers in psychology, 8, 1.012. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01012
  24. Sanz-Aznar, J., Caballero-Molina, J. J. (2021). Aproximación a la imagen subliminal en su uso cinematográfico: modalidades, usos y umbrales. En A. M. de Vicente Domínguez y J. Sierra Sánchez (eds.), La representación audiovisual de la ciencia en el entorno digital (pp. 287-306). Madrid: McGraw Hill.
  25. Siety, E. (2001). Le plan. Au commencement du cinéma. París: Cahiers du cinéma.
  26. Terpou, B., Densmore, M., Thome, J., Frewen, P., Mckinnon, M., Lanius, R. (2019). The innate alarm system and subliminal threat presentation in posttraumatic stress disorder: neuroimaging of the midbrain and cerebellum. Chronic Stress, 3, 1-13. https://doi.org/10.1177/2470547018821496
  27. Van den Bussche, E., Van den Noortgate, W., Reynvoet, B. (2009). Mechanisms of masked priming: a meta-analysis. Psychological bulletin, 135(3), 452-477.
  28. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015329
  29. Vodrahalli, K., Chen, P. H., Liang, Y., Baldassano, C., Chen, J., Yong, E., Honey, C., Hasson, U., Ramadge, P., Norman, K. A., Arora, S. (2018). Mapping between fMRI responses to movies and their natural language annotations. Neuroimage, 180, 223-231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.06.042
  30. Wang, Y., La, Y., Liu, D., Zou, M., Zhang, B., Wang, Y. (2019). The role of response readiness in subliminal visuomotor processes. Consciousness and cognition, 68, 23-32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2018.12.002
  31. Williams, L., Liddell, B., Kemp, A., Bryant, R., Meares, R., Peduto, A., Gordon, E. (2006). Amygdala–prefrontal dissociation of subliminal and supraliminal fear. Human brain mapping, 27(8), 652-661. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20208